READING PASSAGE 3
You
should spend about 20 minutes on Questions 27-40 which are based on Reading
Passage 3 below.
PLAY IS A
SERIOUS BUSINESS
Does
play help develop bigger, better brains? Bryant Furlow investigates
|
A
|
Playing is a serious business.
Children engrossed in a make-believe world, fox cubs play-fighting or
kittens teaming a ball of string aren’t just having fun. Play may look like
a carefree and exuberant way to pass the time before the hard work of
adulthood comes along, but there’s much more to it than that. For a start,
play can even cost animals their lives. Eighty percent of deaths among
juvenile fur seals occur because playing pups fail to sport predators
approaching. It is also extremely expensive in terms of energy. Playful
young animals use around two or three per cent of energy cavorting, and in
children that figure can be closer to fifteen per cent. ‘Even two or three
per cent is huge,’ says John Byers of Idaho University. ‘You just don’t
find animals wasting energy like that,’ he adds. There must be a reason.
|
B
|
But if play is not simply a
developmental hiccup, as biologists once thought, why did it evolve? The
latest idea suggests that play has evolved to build big brains. In other
words, playing makes you intelligent. Playfulness, it seems, is common only
among mammals, although a few of the larger-brained birds also indulge.
Animals at play often use unique signs – tail-wagging in dogs, for example
– to indicate that activity superficially resembling adult behavior is not
really in earnest. In popular explanation of play has been that it helps
juveniles develop the skills they will need to hunt, mate and socialise as
adults. Another has been that it allows young animals to get in shape for
adult life by improving their respiratory endurance. Both these ideas have
been questioned in recent years.
|
C
|
Take the exercise theory. If
play evolved to build muscle or as a kind of endurance training, then you
would expect to see permanent benefits. But Byers points out that the
benefits of increased exercise disappear rapidly after training stops, so
many improvement in endurance resulting from juvenile play would be lost by
adulthood. ‘If the function of play was to get into shape,’ says Byers,
‘the optimum time for playing would depend on when it was most advantageous
for the young of a particular species to do so. But it doesn’t work like
that.’ Across species, play tends to peak about halfway through the
suckling stage and then decline.
|
D
|
Then there’s the skills-
training hypothesis. At first glance, playing animals do appear to be
practising the complex manoeuvres they will need in adulthood. But a closer
inspection reveals this interpretation as too simplistic. In one study,
behavioural ecologist Tim Caro, from the University of California, looked
at the predatory play of kittens and their predatory behaviour when they
reached adulthood. He found that the way the cats played had no significant
effect on their hunting prowess in later life.
|
E
|
Earlier this year, Sergio
Pellis of Lethbridge University, Canada, reported that there is a strong
positive link between brain size and playfulness among mammals in general.
Comparing measurements for fifteen orders of mammals, he and his team found
large brains (for a given body size) are linked to greater playfulness. The
converse was also found to be true. Robert Barton of Durham University
believes that, because large brains are more sensitive to developmental
stimuli than smaller brains, they require more play to help mould them for
adulthood. ‘I concluded it’s to do with learning, and with the importance
of environmental data to the brain during development,’ he says.
|
F
|
According to Byers, the timing
of the playful stage in young animals provides an important clue to what’s
going on. If you plot the amount of time juvenile devotes to play each day
over the course of its development, you discover a pattern typically
associated with a ‘sensitive period’ – a brief development window during
which the brain can actually be modified in ways that are not possible
earlier or later in life. Think of the relative ease with which young
children – but not infants or adults – absorb language. Other researchers
have found that play in cats, rats and mice is at its most intense just as
this ‘window of opportunity” reaches its peak.
|
G
|
‘People have not paid enough
attention to the amount of the brain activated by plays,’ says Marc Bekoff
from Colorado University. Bekoff studied coyote pups at play and found that
the kind of behaviour involved was markedly more variable and unpredictable
than that of adults. Such behaviour activates many different parts of the
brain, he reasons. Bekoff likens it to a behavioural kaleidoscope, with
animals at play jumping rapidly between activities. ‘They use behaviour
from a lot of different contexts – predation, aggression, reproduction,’ he
says. ‘Their developing brain is getting all sorts of stimulation.’
|
H
|
Not only is more of the brain
involved in play that was suspected, but it also seems to activate higher
cognitive processes. ‘There’s enormous cognitive involvement in play,’ says
Bekoff. He points out that play often involves complex assessments of
playmates, ideas of reciprocity and the use of specialised signals and
rules. He believes that play creates a brain that has greater behavioural
flexibility and improved potential for learning later in life. The idea is
backed up by the work of Stephen Siviy of Gettysburg College. Siviy studied
how bouts of play affected the brain’s levels of particular chemical
associated with the stimulation and growth of nerve cells. He was surprised
by the extent of the activation. ‘Play just lights everything up,’ he says.
By allowing link-ups between brain areas that might not normally
communicate with each other, play may enhance creativity.
|
I
|
What might further
experimentation suggest about the way children are raised in many societies
today? We already know that rat pups denied the chance to play grow smaller
brain components and fail to develop the ability to apply social rules when
they interact with their peers. With schooling beginning earlier and
becoming increasingly exam-orientated, play is likely to get even less of a
look-in. Who knows what the result of that will be?
|
Questions 27-32
Reading
Passage 3 has nine paragraphs labelled A-I.
Which
paragraph contains the following information?
Write
the correct letter A-I in boxes 27-32 on your answer sheet.
NB.
You may use any letter more than once.
27
|
the
way play causes unusual connections in the brain which are beneficial
insights
from recording how much time young animals spend playing
a
description of the physical hazards that can accompany play
a
description of the mental activities which are exercised and developed
during play
the
possible effects that a reduction in play opportunities will have on humans
the
classes of animals for which play is important
|
28
|
29
|
30
|
31
|
32
|
Questions 33-35
Choose
THREE letters A-F.
Write
your answers in boxes 33-35 on your answer sheet.
The
list below gives some ways of regarding play.
Which
THREE ways are mentioned by the writer of the text?
|
A
|
a
rehearsal for later adult activities
a
method animals use to prove themselves to their peer group
an
activity intended to build up strength for adulthood
a
means of communicating feelings
a
defensive strategy
an
activity assisting organ growth
|
B
|
C
|
D
|
E
|
F
|
Questions 36-40
Look
at the following researchers (Questions 36-40) and the list of findings
below.
Match
each researcher with the correct finding.
Write
the correct letter A-H in boxes 36-40 on your answer sheet.
List of Findings
|
A
|
There
is a link between a specific substance in the brain and playing.
Play
provides input concerning physical surroundings.
Varieties
of play can be matched to different stages of evolutionary history.
There
is a tendency for mammals with smaller brains to play less.
Play
is not a form of fitness training for the future.
Some
species of larger-brained birds engage in play.
A
wide range of activities are combined during play.
Play
is a method of teaching survival techniques.
|
B
|
C
|
D
|
E
|
F
|
G
|
H
|
36
|
Robert Barton
Marc Bekoff
John Byers
Sergio Pellis
Stephen Siviy
|
37
|
38
|
39
|
40
|
|
No comments:
Post a Comment
thank you for visiting my blog and for your nice comments